The Science Behind Dreams: Characteristics and Functions Explained

Written by: Written by: Philippe Stenstrom, PhD. Psychologist specialized in sleep medicine at HALEO Clinic.

Published on: September 24, 2024 | Reading time: 🕐 2m

The night-time production of dreams is a scientific puzzle that has generated a vast amount of research and academic debate. In this article, we will briefly examine some of the characteristics and proposed functions of dreaming, as described by researchers in the field. 

The characteristics of dreams

Dreams typically occur from a first-person perspective and in the subjective present (Nielsen & Stenstrom, 2005). Most dreams follow a narrative or story-like structure: we are usually engaged in an activity within a particular situation or context. Dreams are almost always social, often featuring multiple characters that interact with the dreamer. 

When do we dream? 

Although most dreams occur during REM sleep, we can dream in any stage of sleep. Interestingly, vivid dreaming often happens as we are falling asleep (Nielsen et al., 2005), but these dreams are typically forgotten unless something wakes us up. 

What do we dream about? 

Generally, we dream about things that relate to our waking lives. The continuity hypothesis (Bell & Hall, 1971) suggests that the majority of dreams reflect the same conceptions (e.g., about oneself, family, and friends) and personal concerns (e.g., relationships, goals) that shape our waking thoughts. 

Where does dream imagery come from? 

Dreams do not usually replay exact episodes from our lives but instead combine elements from several different memories into a new and sometimes bizarre experience. Research has shown that a single dream can be linked to several distinct memories that are years apart but that are often semantically related (Stenstrom et al., 2012). Other studies have found that dreams tend to incorporate recent memories from the same day (known as the "day-residue effect") or about a week prior (the "dream-lag effect"; Nielsen et al., 2004). 

Curiously, we often fail to recognize the oddity of what is happening in a dream. Some researchers (e.g., Hobson et al., 2000) attribute this to the brain being in a different state during sleep, with reduced activity in pre-frontal areas responsible for higher-order thinking, which may prevent the dreamer from realizing how unlikely or impossible the dream scenarios are. 

 

The functions of dreaming

It remains a scientific mystery why we dream and whether dreaming serves a specific function. While some researchers believe that dreams are simply by-products of brain activity (Hobson et al., 2000), others have proposed interesting hypotheses suggesting functional roles for dreams. 

Dreams and creativity

Numerous anecdotes suggest that dreaming enhances creativity and problem-solving skills among artists and scientists. For example, John Lennon reportedly came up with the melody for "Yesterday" in a dream, and the discovery of the molecular structure of benzene was inspired by a dream of a snake biting its own tail. Both Salvador Dali and Thomas Edison used sleep onset dreams for inspiration and problem-solving, employing a similar method to wake themselves up from the edge of sleep to remember their dream. 

The connection between dreams and creativity has been widely debated in the scientific community. While some studies suggest that dreaming involves greater associative divergence, recent research (Horowitz et al., 2023) has found direct evidence of dream content enhancing creative performance. 

Dreaming and memory consolidation

Newly learned information frequently appears in dreams, suggesting a connection between dream content and memory consolidation. A recent meta-analysis found that dreaming about a learning task is associated with improved memory performance, indicating that dream content may be related to memory consolidation (Hudachek & Wamsley, 2023).  

The relationship between dreaming and memory processing is intricate and sometimes surprising. For example, a study involving amnesic patients that played the video game Tetris revealed that, despite being unable to recall playing the video game, these individuals described Tetris blocks in their dreams as they fell asleep. 

Dreaming remains a fascinating and complex phenomenon that continues to intrigue scientists. While many aspects of dreams are still debated, research provides insights into their characteristics and possible functions. Dreams often reflect elements of our waking lives, merging fragments of memories into novel narratives, and they may play a role in memory consolidation.
Additionally, dreams have long been associated with creativity, as both anecdotal evidence and scientific studies suggest their potential to enhance problem-solving and inspire new ideas. Although further research is needed to better understand the many facets of dreaming, the current body of knowledge suggests that dreams may serve important cognitive functions. 

 

Need help with your sleep? 

I want HALEO for me

We can help you! HALEO offers a solution that can contribute to productivity and well-being through quality sleep. Schedule a discovery meeting by booking your time here." 

I want to offer HALEO to my organization

 

 

Sources:

Hall, C. S., and Nordby, V. J. (1972). The Individual and His Dreams. New York: New American Library. 
Hudachek L, Wamsley EJ. A meta-analysis of the relation between dream content and memory consolidation. Sleep. 2023 Dec 11;46(12):zsad111. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsad111. PMID: 37058584. 

Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R. Dreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000;23:793–842. discussion 904-1121.  

Horowitz AH, Esfahany K, Gálvez TV, Maes P, Stickgold R. Targeted dream incubation at sleep onset increases post-sleep creative performance. Sci Rep. 2023 May 15;13(1):7319. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-31361-w. PMID: 37188795; PMCID: PMC10185495. 

Revonsuo A. The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav Brain Sci. 2000 Dec;23(6):877-901; discussion 904-1121. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00004015. PMID: 11515147. 

Nielsen TA, Kuiken D, Alain G, Stenstrom P, Powell RA. Immediate and delayed incorporations of events into dreams: further replication and implications for dream function. J Sleep Res. 2004 Dec;13(4):327-36. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00421.x. PMID: 15560767. 

Nielsen TA, Stenstrom P. What are the memory sources of dreaming? Nature. 2005 Oct 27;437(7063):1286-9. doi: 10.1038/nature04288. PMID: 16251954. 

Nielsen T, Stenstrom P, Takeuchi T, Saucier S, Lara-Carrasco J, Solomonova E, Martel E. Partial REM-sleep deprivation increases the dream-like quality of mentation from REM sleep and sleep onset. Sleep. 2005 Sep;28(9):1083-9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.9.1083. PMID: 16268377. 

Stenstrom, P., Fox, K., Solomonova, E., & Nielsen, T. (2012). Mentation during sleep onset theta bursts in a trained participant: A role for NREM stage 1 sleep in memory processing? International Journal of Dream Research, 5(1), 37–46. 

Stickgold R, Malia A, Maguire D, Roddenberry D, O'Connor M. Replaying the game: hypnagogic images in normals and amnesics. Science. 2000 Oct 13;290(5490):350-3. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5490.350. PMID: 11030656. 

M126 - More Articles